Back to Penobscot Bay Watch

NEXT PAGE

Page 9

been observed in a number of cod stocks including the Labrador-East Newfoundland complex and southern Gulf of St. Lawrence stock. In the latter region, the area occupied by age groups 3-8+ cod increased as abundance increased (Swain and Wade 1993). In comparison to the older cod, age-3 were more spatially restricted at low population size, their range expanded more slowly as abundance increased, and changes in relative density among parts of the Gulf were smaller between years of low- and high-abundance. Younger juveniles were thought to experience less severe competitive pressures for food or wider variation in habitat quality than the older age-groups.

A behavioral theory applied to explain the pattem of geographic distribution is density- dependent habitat use. This hypothesis was applied to young cod in coastal habitats (Olsen and Soldal 1989) where catches of post-settlement juveniles showed a high degree of small-scale spatial consistency regardless of cohort size. In years of high year-class abundance, density increases to an upper limit in the most suitable habitat and as the fitness of individuals occupying the prime sites declines due to intraspecific competition, diffusion to and use of suboptimal habitat expands. Accordingly, at low population size, individuals occupy habitat with high basic foraging and protective suitability.

The theory was tested for the Labrador-East Newfoundland stock complex for which contraction has been confirmed for adult cod at low stock size (Taggart et al. 1994; Atkinson et al. 1997). Catches of age-groups 0-2 were analyzed from 1959-64 and 1992-94 at a series of fixed sampling sites extending over 1,500 miles of Newfoundland coastline (Schneider et al. 1997). In years of low cohort size, contraction did not occur in coastal habitats, i.e., density of juvenile cod was independent of area within the occupied <20 m depth range. They noted that sampling sites with high densities in some years had low densities in years of high abundance, an observation inconsistent with spillover theory in good years.

In support of density-dependent theory, high post-settlement densities of age-0 cod were found in eelgrass beds of Trinity Bay, Newfoundland, during 1994 and 1995, years of good and bad year- classes, respectively; however, a significant increase in abundance in less suitable no-eelgrass habitat was noted in 1994 when settlement strength was high (Grant and Brown 1998a). The high 1994 densities in less-utilized no-eelgrass habitat during a year of high abundance would be consistent with the hypothesis of density-dependent habitat use or selection. The researchers acknowledged that their observations were on a small temporal and spatial scale. Re-analysis of the fixed sampling site juvenile catch data from Newfoundland showed a stronger recruitment signal from a small number of sites visited frequently than the entire set of sites (Ings et al. 1997). The 1994 year class was ranked significantly stronger than the three previous year- classes following stock collapse in a broad-scale study (Anderson and Dalley 1997). On the other hand, there was no evidence of fewer settled 0-group juveniles anywhere along the coast in 1995 relative to the 1992-94 year-classes (Smedbol et al. 1998).

For a number of cod stocks, variability in year class strength is usually determined in the larval stage and attenuated by density-dependent juvenile mortality (Myers and Cadigan 1993a). Biological processes that may result in density-dependent mortality would include:

(1) competition for food with mortality resulting from increased predation or starvation;
(2) intercohort cannibalism; (3) predators switching to abundant year-classes; and (4) a

Page 9

NEXT PAGE